Skip to main content
Statistics LibreTexts

Causation and Experiments

  • Page ID
    31293
  • \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\) \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    ( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\) \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\) \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\) \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

    \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\id}{\mathrm{id}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\kernel}{\mathrm{null}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\range}{\mathrm{range}\,}\)

    \( \newcommand{\RealPart}{\mathrm{Re}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\ImaginaryPart}{\mathrm{Im}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Argument}{\mathrm{Arg}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\norm}[1]{\| #1 \|}\)

    \( \newcommand{\inner}[2]{\langle #1, #2 \rangle}\)

    \( \newcommand{\Span}{\mathrm{span}}\) \( \newcommand{\AA}{\unicode[.8,0]{x212B}}\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorA}[1]{\vec{#1}}      % arrow\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorAt}[1]{\vec{\text{#1}}}      % arrow\)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorB}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorC}[1]{\textbf{#1}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorD}[1]{\overrightarrow{#1}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectorDt}[1]{\overrightarrow{\text{#1}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vectE}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash{\mathbf {#1}}}} \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecs}[1]{\overset { \scriptstyle \rightharpoonup} {\mathbf{#1}} } \)

    \( \newcommand{\vecd}[1]{\overset{-\!-\!\rightharpoonup}{\vphantom{a}\smash {#1}}} \)

    CO-3: Describe the strengths and limitations of designed experiments and observational studies.

    Learning Objectives

    LO 3.2: Explain how the study design impacts the types of conclusions that can be drawn.

    Learning Objectives

    LO 3.3: Identify and define key features of experimental design (randomized, blind etc.).

    Video

    Video: Causation and Experiments (8:57)

    Recall that in an experiment, it is the researchers who assign values of the explanatory variable to the participants. The key to ensuring that individuals differ only with respect to explanatory values — which is also the key to establishing causation — lies in the way this assignment is carried out. Let’s return to the smoking cessation study as a context to explore the essential ingredients of experimental design.

    EXAMPLE:

    In our discussion of the distinction between observational studies and experiments, we described the following experiment: collect a representative sample of 1,000 individuals from the population of smokers who are just now trying to quit. We divide the sample into 4 groups of 250 and instruct each group to use a different method to quit. One year later, we contact the same 1,000 individuals and determine whose attempts succeeded while using our designated method.

    This was an experiment, because the researchers themselves determined the values of the explanatory variable of interest for the individuals studied, rather than letting them choose.

    We will begin by using the context of this smoking cessation example to illustrate the specialized vocabulary of experiments. First of all, the explanatory variable, or factor, in this case is the method used to quit. The different imposed values of the explanatory variable, or treatments (common abbreviation: ttt), consist of the four possible quitting methods. The groups receiving different treatments are called treatment groups. The group that tries to quit without drugs or therapy could be called the control group — those individuals on whom no specific treatment was imposed. Ideally, the subjects (human participants in an experiment) in each treatment group differ from those in the other treatment groups only with respect to the treatment (quitting method). As mentioned in our discussion of why lurking variables prevent us from establishing causation in observational studies, eliminating all other differences among treatment groups will be the key to asserting causation via an experiment. How can this be accomplished?

    Randomized Controlled Experiments

    Your intuition may already tell you, correctly, that random assignment to treatments is the best way to prevent treatment groups of individuals from differing from each other in ways other than the treatment assigned. Either computer software or tables can be utilized to accomplish the random assignment. The resulting design is called a randomized controlled experiment, because researchers control values of the explanatory variable with a randomization procedure. Under random assignment, the groups should not differ significantly with respect to any potential lurking variable. Then, if we see a relationship between the explanatory and response variables, we have evidence that it is a causal one.

    A visual representation of the experimental study. A large circle represents the entire population. Through random selection we generate the sample, which is represented as a smaller circle. The circle representing the samples is divided up evenly into 4 pieces, each piece representing one value of the explanatory variable (method). The pieces are treatment groups randomly assigned by researchers.

    Comments:

    • Note that in a randomized controlled experiment, a randomization procedure may be used in two phases. First, a sample of subjects is collected. Ideally it would be a random sample so that it would be perfectly representative of the entire population.
    • Often researchers have no choice but to recruit volunteers. Using volunteers may help to offset one of the drawbacks to experimentation which will be discussed later, namely the problem of noncompliance.
    • Second, we assign individuals randomly to the treatment groups to ensure that the only difference between them will be due to the treatment and we can get evidence of causation. At this stage, randomization is vital.

    Let’s discuss some other issues related to experimentation.

    Inclusion of a Control Group

    A common misconception is that an experiment must include a control group of individuals receiving no treatment. There may be situations where a complete lack of treatment is not an option, or where including a control group is ethically questionable, or where researchers explore the effects of a treatment without making a comparison. Here are a few examples:

    EXAMPLE:

    If doctors want to conduct an experiment to determine whether Prograf or Cyclosporin is more effective as an immunosuppressant, they could randomly assign transplant patients to take one or the other of the drugs. It would, of course, be unethical to include a control group of patients not receiving any immunosuppressants.

    EXAMPLE:

    Recently, experiments have been conducted in which the treatment is a highly invasive brain surgery. The only way to have a legitimate control group in this case is to randomly assign half of the subjects to undergo the entire surgery except for the actual treatment component (inserting stem cells into the brain). This, of course, is also ethically problematic (but, believe it or not, is being done).

    EXAMPLE:

    There may even be an experiment designed with only a single treatment. For example, makers of a new hair product may ask a sample of individuals to treat their hair with that product over a period of several weeks, then assess how manageable their hair has become. Such a design is clearly flawed because of the absence of a comparison group, but it is still an experiment because use of the product has been imposed by its manufacturers, rather than chosen naturally by the individuals. A flawed experiment is nevertheless an experiment.

    Comment:

    • The word control is used in at least three different senses.
      • In the context of observational studies, we control for a confounding variable by separating it out.
      • Referring to an experiment as a controlled experiment stresses that the values of the experiment’s explanatory variables (factors) have been assigned by researchers, as opposed to having occurred naturally.
      • In the context of experiments, the control group consists of subjects who do not receive a treatment, but who are otherwise handled identically to those who do receive the treatment.

    Learn By Doing: Random Assignment to Treatment Groups (Software)

    Blind and Double-Blind Experiments

    Suppose the experiment about methods for quitting smoking were carried out with randomized assignments of subjects to the four treatments, and researchers determined that the percentage succeeding with the combination drug/therapy method was highest, and the percentage succeeding with no drugs or therapy was lowest. In other words, suppose there is clear evidence of an association between method used and success rate. Could it be concluded that the drug/therapy method causes success more than trying to quit without using drugs or therapy? Perhaps.

    Although randomized controlled experiments do give us a better chance of pinning down the effects of the explanatory variable of interest, they are not completely problem-free. For example, suppose that the manufacturers of the smoking cessation drug had just launched a very high-profile advertising campaign with the goal of convincing people that their drug is extremely effective as a method of quitting.

    Even with a randomized assignment to treatments, there would be an important difference among subjects in the four groups: those in the drug and combination drug/therapy groups would perceive their treatment as being a promising one, and may be more likely to succeed just because of added confidence in the success of their assigned method. Therefore, the ideal circumstance is for the subjects to be unaware of which treatment is being administered to them: in other words, subjects in an experiment should be (if possible) blind to which treatment they received.

    How could researchers arrange for subjects to be blind when the treatment involved is a drug? They could administer a placebo pill to the control group, so that there are no psychological differences between those who receive the drug and those who do not. The word “placebo” is derived from a Latin word that means “to please.” It is so named because of the natural tendency of human subjects to improve just because of the “pleasing” idea of being treated, regardless of the benefits of the treatment itself. When patients improve because they are told they are receiving treatment, even though they are not actually receiving treatment, this is known as the placebo effect.

    Next, how could researchers arrange for subjects to be blind when the treatment involved is a type of therapy? This is more problematic. Clearly, subjects must be aware of whether they are undergoing some type of therapy or not. There is no practical way to administer a “placebo” therapy to some subjects. Thus, the relative success of the drug/therapy treatment may be due to subjects’ enhanced confidence in the success of the method they happened to be assigned. We may feel fairly certain that the method itself causes success in quitting, but we cannot be absolutely sure.

    When the response of interest is fairly straightforward, such as giving up cigarettes or not, then recording its values is a simple process in which researchers need not use their own judgment in making an assessment. There are many experiments where the response of interest is less definite, such as whether or not a cancer patient has improved, or whether or not a psychiatric patient is less depressed. In such cases, it is important for researchers who evaluate the response to be blind to which treatment the subject received, in order to prevent the experimenter effect from influencing their assessments. If neither the subjects nor the researchers know who was assigned what treatment, then the experiment is called double-blind.

    The most reliable way to determine whether the explanatory variable is actually causing changes in the response variable is to carry out a randomized controlled double-blind experiment. Depending on the variables of interest, such a design may not be entirely feasible, but the closer researchers get to achieving this ideal design, the more convincing their claims of causation (or lack thereof) are.

    Did I Get This?: Experiments

    Pitfalls in Experimentation

    Some of the inherent difficulties that may be encountered in experimentation are the Hawthorne effect, lack of realism, noncompliance, and treatments that are unethical, impossible, or impractical to impose.

    We already introduced a hypothetical experiment to determine if people tend to snack more while they watch TV:

    • Recruit participants for the study.
    • While they are presumably waiting to be interviewed, half of the individuals sit in a waiting room with snacks available and a TV on. The other half sit in a waiting room with snacks available and no TV, just magazines.
    • Researchers determine whether people consume more snacks in the TV setting.

    Suppose that, in fact, the subjects who sat in the waiting room with the TV consumed more snacks than those who sat in the room without the TV. Could we conclude that in their everyday lives, and in their own homes, people eat more snacks when the TV is on? Not necessarily, because people’s behavior in this very controlled setting may be quite different from their ordinary behavior.

    If they suspect their snacking behavior is being observed, they may alter their behavior, either consciously or subconsciously. This phenomenon, whereby people in an experiment behave differently from how they would normally behave, is called the Hawthorne effect. Even if they don’t suspect they are being observed in the waiting room, the relationship between TV and snacking in the waiting room might not be representative of what it is in real life.

    One of the greatest advantages of an experiment — that researchers take control of the explanatory variable — can also be a disadvantage in that it may result in a rather unrealistic setting. Lack of realism (also called lack of ecological validity) is a possible drawback to the use of an experiment rather than an observational study to explore a relationship. Depending on the explanatory variable of interest, it may be quite easy or it may be virtually impossible to take control of the variable’s values and still maintain a fairly natural setting.

    In our hypothetical smoking cessation example, both the observational study and the experiment were carried out on a random sample of 1,000 smokers with intentions to quit. In the case of the observational study, it would be reasonably feasible to locate 1,000 such people in the population at large, identify their intended method, and contact them again a year later to establish whether they succeeded or not.

    In the case of the experiment, it is not so easy to take control of the explanatory variable (cessation method) merely by telling all 1,000 subjects what method they must use. Noncompliance (failure to submit to the assigned treatment) could enter in on such a large scale as to render the results invalid.

    In order to ensure that the subjects in each treatment group actually undergo the assigned treatment, researchers would need to pay for the treatment and make it easily available. The cost of doing that for a group of 1,000 people would go beyond the budget of most researchers.

    Even if the drugs or therapy were paid for, it is very unlikely that most of the subjects contacted at random would be willing to use a method not of their own choosing, but dictated by the researchers. From a practical standpoint, such a study would most likely be carried out on a smaller group of volunteers, recruited via flyers or some other sort of advertisement.

    The fact that they are volunteers might make them somewhat different from the larger population of smokers with intentions to quit, but it would reduce the more worrisome problem of non-compliance. Volunteers may have a better overall chance of success, but if researchers are primarily concerned with which method is most successful, then the relative success of the various methods should be roughly the same for the volunteer sample as it would be for the general population, as long as the methods are randomly assigned. Thus, the most vital stage for randomization in an experiment is during the assignment of treatments, rather than the selection of subjects.

    There are other, more serious drawbacks to experimentation, as illustrated in the following hypothetical examples:

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose researchers want to determine if the drug Ecstasy causes memory loss. One possible design would be to take a group of volunteers and randomly assign some to take Ecstasy on a regular basis, while the others are given a placebo. Test them periodically to see if the Ecstasy group experiences more memory problems than the placebo group.

    The obvious flaw in this experiment is that it is unethical (and actually also illegal) to administer a dangerous drug like Ecstasy, even if the subjects are volunteers. The only feasible design to seek answers to this particular research question would be an observational study.

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose researchers want to determine whether females wash their hair more frequently than males.

    It is impossible to assign some subjects to be female and others male, and so an experiment is not an option here. Again, an observational study would be the only way to proceed.

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose researchers want to determine whether being in a lower income bracket may be responsible for obesity in women, at least to some extent, because they can’t afford more nutritious meals and don’t have the means to participate in fitness activities.

    The socioeconomic status of the study subject is a variable that cannot be controlled by the researchers, so an experiment is impossible. (Even if the researchers could somehow raise the money to provide a random sample of women with substantial salaries, the effects of their eating habits during their lives before the study began would still be present, and would affect the study’s outcome.)

    These examples should convince you that, depending on the variables of interest, researching their relationship via an experiment may be too unrealistic, unethical, or impractical. Observational studies are subject to flaws, but often they are the only recourse.

    Let’s summarize what we’ve learned so far:

    1. Observational studies:

    • The explanatory variable’s values are allowed to occur naturally.
    • Because of the possibility of lurking variables, it is difficult to establish causation.
    • If possible, control for suspected lurking variables by studying groups of similar individuals separately.
    • Some lurking variables are difficult to control for; others may not be identified.

    2. Experiments

    • The explanatory variable’s values are controlled by researchers (treatment is imposed).
    • Randomized assignment to treatments automatically controls for all lurking variables.
    • Making subjects blind avoids the placebo effect.
    • Making researchers blind avoids conscious or subconscious influences on their subjective assessment of responses.
    • A randomized controlled double-blind experiment is generally optimal for establishing causation.
    • A lack of realism may prevent researchers from generalizing experimental results to real-life situations.
    • Noncompliance may undermine an experiment. A volunteer sample might solve (at least partially) this problem.
    • It is impossible, impractical, or unethical to impose some treatments.

    More About Experiments

    CO-3: Describe the strengths and limitations of designed experiments and observational studies.

    Learning Objectives

    LO 3.2: Explain how the study design impacts the types of conclusions that can be drawn.

    Learning Objectives

    LO 3.3: Identify and define key features of experimental design (randomized, blind etc.).

    Video

    Video: More About Experiments (4:09)

    Experiments With More Than One Explanatory Variable

    It is not uncommon for experiments to feature two or more explanatory variables (called factors). In this course, we focus on exploratory data analysis and statistical inference in situations which involve only one explanatory variable. Nevertheless, we will now consider the design for experiments involving several explanatory variables, in order to familiarize students with their basic structure.

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose researchers are not only interested in the effect of diet on blood pressure, but also the effect of two new drugs. Subjects are assigned to either Control Diet (no restrictions), Diet #1, or Diet #2, (the variable diet has, then, 3 possible values) and are also assigned to receive either Placebo, Drug #1, or Drug #2 (the variable Drug, then, also has three values). This is an example where the experiment has two explanatory variables and a response variable. In order to set up such an experiment, there has to be one treatment group for every combination of categories of the two explanatory variables. Thus, in this case there are 3 * 3 = 9 combinations of the two variables to which the subjects are assigned. The treatment groups are illustrated and labeled in the following table:

    The column headings for the table are for the Diet variable: "No-diet", "Special diet 1" and "Special diet 2." The Rows are for the drug variable: "Placebo," "Drug 1," and "Drug 2." There are 9 cells in the table, one for every possible combination of row and column. These cells are labeled "tttX", where X is in the range of [1-9], representing each combination.

    Subjects would be randomly assigned to one of the nine treatment groups. If we find differences in the proportions of subjects who achieve the lower “moderate zone” blood pressure among the nine treatment groups, then we have evidence that the diets and/or drugs may be effective for reducing blood pressure.

    From the population we generate a sample. The individuals of the sample are represented as a whole visually with a circle. These individuals are then divided by randomly assigning them to one of the 9 treatment groups. These treatment groups are "ttt1: no-diet and placebo,", "ttt2: diet 1 and placebo", "ttt3: diet 2 and placebo", and so on, up to "ttt9: diet 2 and drug 2." The responses from each of these treatment groups are compared.

    Comments:

    • Recall that randomization may be employed at two stages of an experiment: in the selection of subjects, and in the assignment of treatments. The former may be helpful in allowing us to generalize what occurs among our subjects to what would occur in the general population, but the reality of most experimental settings is that a convenience or volunteer sample is used. Most likely the blood pressure study described above would use volunteer subjects. The important thing is to make sure these subjects are randomly assigned to one of the nine treatment combinations.
    • In order to gain optimal information about individuals in all the various treatment groups, we would like to make assignments not just randomly, but also evenly. If there are 90 subjects in the blood pressure study described above, and 9 possible treatment groups, then each group should be filled randomly with 10 individuals. A simple random sample of 10 could be taken from the larger group of 90, and those individuals would be assigned to the first treatment group. Next, the second treatment group would be filled by a simple random sample of 10 taken from the remaining 80 subjects. This process would be repeated until all 9 groups are filled with 10 individuals each.

    Did I Get This?: Experiments #2

    Modifications to Randomization

    In some cases, an experiment’s design may be enhanced by relaxing the requirement of total randomization and blocking the subjects first, dividing them into groups of individuals who are similar with respect to an outside variable that may be important in the relationship being studied. This can help ensure that the effect of treatments, as well as background variables, are most precisely measured. In blocking, we simply split the sampled subjects into blocks based upon the different values of the background variable, and then randomly allocate treatments within each block. Thus, blocking in the assignment of subjects is analogous to stratification in sampling.

    For example, consider again our experiment examining the differences between three versions of software from the last Learn By Doing activity. If we suspected that gender might affect individuals’ software preferences, we might choose to allocate subjects to separate blocks, one for males and one for females. Within each block, subjects are randomly assigned to treatments and the treatment proceeds as usual. A diagram of blocking in this situation is below:

    We have 2 blocks, 3 treatment groups each (by random assignment). From the population we generate a sample. This sample of individuals is then split into two blocks, Males and Females. Each block is then randomly split further into the three treatment groups: "tt1: existing software," "ttt2 new software 1," and "ttt3 new software 2." So, we end up with 6 total groups. Within each group the responses from the treatment groups are compared to each other, generating results separately for each block.

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose producers of gasoline want to compare which of two types of gas results in better mileage for automobiles. In case the size of the vehicle plays a role in the effectiveness of different types of gasoline, they could first block by vehicle size, then randomly assign some cars within each block to Gasoline A and others to Gasoline B:

    This example consists of 2 blocks, 2 treatment groups each (by random assignment). From the population we generate a sample, then separate it into two blocks, "Small" and "Large," according to the vehicle size.; Within these blocks we randomly assign vehicles to use either Gasoline A or Gasoline B (So, each block is split into two treatment groups, "ttt1: Gasoline A", and "ttt2: Gasoline B"), resulting in 4 total groups. Then, within each block, we compare the responses, so we obtain results for each block individually.

    In the extreme, researchers may examine a relationship for a sample of blocks of just two individuals who are similar in many important respects, or even the same individual whose responses are compared for two explanatory values.

    EXAMPLE:

    For example, researchers could compare the effects of Gasoline A and Gasoline B when both are used on the same car, for a sample of many cars of various sizes and models.

    In this Matched Pairs Design we have n blocks of individual cars, with 2 treatment groups each, done by random assignment. From the population we generate the sample group. The sample group is then placed into n blocks for each individual car. Each of these blocks is subjected to two treatments by random assignment. These treatments are "ttt1 Gasoline A" and "ttt2 Gasoline B." For each car, the responses to each treatment are compared, resulting in a treatment for each

    Such a study design, called matched pairs, may enable us to pinpoint the effects of the explanatory variable by comparing responses for the same individual under two explanatory values, or for two individuals who are as similar as possible except that the first gets one treatment, and the second gets another (or serves as the control). Treatments should usually be assigned at random within each pair, or the order of treatments should be randomized for each individual. In our gasoline example, for each car the order of testing (Gasoline A first, or Gasoline B first) should be randomized.

    EXAMPLE:

    Suppose researchers want to compare the relative merits of toothpastes with and without tartar control ingredients. In order to make the comparison between individuals who are as similar as possible with respect to background and diet, they could obtain a sample of identical twins. One of each pair would randomly be assigned to brush with the tartar control toothpaste, while the other would brush with regular toothpaste of the same brand. These would be provided in unmarked tubes, so that the subjects would be blind. To make the experiment double-blind, dentists who evaluate the results would not know who used which toothpaste.

    Paired Design. There are n blocks, each represented by a circle with two identical twins in them. Randomly, the treatment of tartar or regular toothpaste is given to each twin. So, each circle has two twins, two types of toothpaste, and each twin randomly gets assigned one type of toothpaste.

    “Before-and-after” studies are another common type of matched pairs design. For each individual, the response variable of interest is measured twice: first before the treatment, then again after the treatment. The categorical explanatory variable is which treatment was applied, or whether a treatment was applied, to that participant.

    Comment:

    • We have explained data production as a two-stage process: first obtain the sample, then evaluate the variables of interest via an appropriate study design. Even though the steps are carried out in this order chronologically, it is generally best for researchers to decide on a study design before they actually obtain the sample. For the toothpaste example above, researchers would first decide to use the matched pairs design, then obtain a sample of identical twins, then carry out the experiment and assess the results.

    These examples should convince you that, depending on the variables of interest, researching their relationship via an experiment may be too unrealistic, unethical, or impractical. Observational studies are subject to flaws, but often they are the only recourse.

    Did I Get This?: More About Experiments


    Causation and Experiments is shared under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 license and was authored, remixed, and/or curated by LibreTexts.

    • Was this article helpful?